Download MP3s: - Paradoxal Melody.aif (438k) - Paradoxal Scale.aif (310k) --| Paradoxal Music |--- The endless staircase stands as a classic visual paradox, continuously tricking the eye into a geometrically impossible journey. First devised in the 1950Õs by l.S. Penrose and roger penrose of the university of london and later made famous by the dutch artist m.C. Escher, this paradox has a rich set of acoustic counterparts. In the early 1960Õs roger n. Shepard of bell telephone laboratories produced a rather remarkable example. He repeatedly played the same sequence of computer generated tones that moved up in an octave. Instead of hearing the pattern stop and then start again, listeners heard the pattern descend endlessly. ...The physical basis of musical pitch has fascinated scientists since antiquity. Pythagoras established that the pitch of a vibrating string varies inversely with its length: the shorter the string, the higher the pitch. He also demonstrated that certain musical intervals Ð the pitch relation between two tones Ð correspond to ratios formed by different lengths of string. In the 17th century galileo and the french mathematician and theologian marin mersenne showed that the basis of these associations lay in the relation between string length and frequency of vibration. Mersennse also demonstrated the existence of overtones, or harmonics, in vibrating bodies. That is, a vibration occurs both at the frequency corresponding to the perceived pitch (the fundamental frequency) and at frequencies that are whole number multiples of the fundamental (harmonics). In other words, a tone whose fundamental frequency is 100 hertz contains components at 200 hertz, 300 hertz, 400 hertz and so on. In the 1930s jan schouten of philips laboratory in eindhoven showed that the auditory system exploits this phenomenon. When presented with a harmonic series, we can perceive a pitch taht corresponds to the fundamental frequency, even if the fundamental itself is missing. The relations between pitches enable us to hear musical patterns. When two tones are presented simultaneously or in succession, we perceive a musical interval. Intervals are heard to be the same in size when the fundamental frequencies of their component tones stand in the same ratio. (Technically, the tones within each pair are separated by the same distance in log frequency.) This principle forms one of the bases of the traditional musical scale. The smallest unit of this scale is the semi-tone, which is the pitch relation formed by two adjacent notes on a keyboard. The semitone corresponds to a frequency ratio of approximiately 18:17. Intervals composed of the same number of semitones are given the same name. For example, the interval corresponding to a ratio of 2:1 (12 semitones) is termed an octave, the interval corresponding to a ratio of 3:2 (seven semitones) is termed a fifth and the ratio 4:3 (five semitones) is called a fourth. Tones related by octaves are in a sense perceptually equivalent. Each of the 12 semitones in an octave is assigned a name (c, c#, d and so on). The entire scale (called the chormatic scale) consists of note names across octaves. The note names are identified by subscripts for example, middle c can be written as c4. The c one octave lower is c3, and the one above is c5. The pitch of a tone can thus be regarded as varying along two dimensions. The first, known as pitch height, extends from low to high, which can experience by sweeping a hand all the way up a keyboard. The second is the circular dimension of pitch class, which defines a toneÕs position within the octave. Researchers call this dimension as the pitch class circle. The circle leads to an immediate assumption: it is nonsensical to ask whether one tone, say, C, is higher than another, such as f#. To clarify the question, one would need to give the octaves in which the two tones occur. In the absence of such information the human brain still tries to organise tones so that it can judge relative pitch. Shepard demonstrated this phenomenon in 1964. Using a music synthesis programme developed by his colleague max mathews, he generated a series of tones that were clearly defined in terms of pitch class but in which the octave containing the tones was unclear. Each tone consisted of sinusoidal components (smoothly oscillating waves) separated by octaves, so that the tones were composed only of harmonics in the same pitch class. Shepard found that when two such tones were played, one after the other, subjects heard either an ascending pattern or a descending one. The perceived direction depended on the distance seperating the two tones along the pitch class circle: listeners followed the shorter distance between the tones. For example, subjects heard the pair c#-d as ascending, because the shorter distance here is clockwise. Analoguously, the pair a-g# was always heard as descending. This finding enabled shepard to produce the striking demonstration described at the beginning of this article. A series of tones repeatedly traverses the pitch class circle in clockwise steps appears to ascend endlessly in pitch. If the series of tones traverses the circle in counterclockwise steps, it appears to descend infinitely. Jean-claude risset, now at the laboratory for mechanics and acoustics at the cnrs in marseilles [france], produced an intriguing variant. He created a single tone that glided around the pitch class circle in a clockwise direction. The tone appeared to ascend endlessly [just like i heard in that *slowdive* song :) ] Érisset used such a gliding tone when he composed incidental music to pierre haletÕs [play, the] *little boy*É Éin my laboratory i recently created pitch circularity effects using a set of tones, each of which constituted a full harmonic series but in which the relative amplitudes (loudnesses) of the harmonics were such as to generate ambiguities of perceived pitch height. Listeners obtained an impression of a series that ascended infinitely in pitch. These demonstrations of pitch circularity illustrate that the human mind tends to form linkages between elements that are close together rather than those that are far apart. Analogous phenomena can be found in vision. For example, we tend to group together dots that are next to one another and to perceive movement between neighbouring lights turned on and off in succession. [Like the movement perceived in the lights in a marquee.] [To envision the pitch class circle, imagine the notes of the chromatic scale arranged around the face of a clock, with the notes resident at the following designations: 12:00-c, 1:00-c#, 2:00-d, 3:00-d#, 4:00-e, 5:00-f, 6:00-f#, 7:00-g, 8:00-g#, 9:00-a, 10:00-a#, 11:00-b, and back to 12:00-c.] Musical paradoxes occur when sequences of tones appear to rise or fall even though the tones lack the physical cues normally used to judge pitch height [this is done by electronically eliminating the fundamental frequency which is ussually heard when a string or conventional instrument is agitated. (?) ] Such paradoxes can be understood in terms of the pitch class circle, which represents the tones in an octave. The tones played are opposite one another along the circle. In an example of this phenomenon, called the tritone paradox, d is played at time t1 followed by g# at t2. Some listeners heard the sequence ascend; others heard it descend. In a variation called the semitone paradox, d# and g# are presented simultaneously, follwed by d and a. Another version, the melodic paradox, uses three pairs of tones. In these cases, some subjects heard the ascending sequence as higher than the descending one, and others heard it as lower. The results show that the subjects must have preferred orientations of the pitch class [depending on locality and dialect of the language they speak] circle with respect to itÕs pitch height [for example, british and americans will hear the tri-tone paradox differently. The british typically hear the same notes ascend that the californians hear as descending.]. (Scientific American, August 1992, Volume 267, Number 2, Paradoxes of Musical Pitch, Diana Deutsch, pp. 88-90, ISSN: 0036-8733.) -- |